top of page

The impact of parental input on Child language development (from the child’s birth to early childhood(First language acquisition perspective), How FLA differ from SLA (Second Language acquisition)?

I. Introduction

Language development is a crucial aspect of child development, as it enables children to comprehend and communicate through speech. The first three years of life, when the brain is developing and maturing, is the most intensive period for acquiring speech and language skills. During this time, children develop the ability to understand and communicate language, which is essential for their learning and social competence.

Language development involves both expressive language (communicating) and receptive language (understanding) skills. These skills are key to children’s learning and social competence, and they are closely related to literacy and the basis for learning to read and write. Milestones of language development, such as the ability to comprehend language, use vocabulary to express ideas, follow directions, and use language to establish and maintain relationships, are important indicators of a child’s progress.

Early language development is crucial as it reflects the growth and maturation of the brain. It is essential to compare a child’s language development with norms rather than with other children, and to encourage early language development by engaging in sustained conversations, following the child’s lead, and providing opportunities for dialogue and communication.

II. Stages of Child Language Development

A. Prenatal Stage

1. Prenatal Language Exposure

During the prenatal stage, infants can recognize and prefer their mother’s voice, and exposure to language rhythms begins before birth. Fetuses can perceive and react to external sounds, including language. Prenatal language exposure has been shown to shape the brain and contribute to the development of language skills in newborns. Research suggests that exposure to language in the womb can enhance a newborn’s sensitivity to the language they have previously heard, and this effect is not limited to the amount or quality of prenatal speech exposure.

2. Emotional Connection through Prenatal Speech

During the prenatal stage, reading books aloud and talking to the unborn child can introduce them to language patterns and create a bond. Research indicates that fetuses can perceive and react to external sounds, including language, and exposure to language in the womb can shape the brain and contribute to the development of language skills in newborns.

B. Infancy (0–2 years)

1. Pre-linguistic Communication

During the infancy stage (0–2 years), infants engage in nonspecific vocalizations such as cooing and babbling, which lay the groundwork for language development. Babies also respond to stimuli and develop turn-taking behaviors, which are early nonverbal responses. Parents who respond positively to babbling encourage the development of speech, and interactive communication helps infants learn the rhythm of conversation.

2. Parental Responsiveness

The search results provide information on parental responsiveness during the stages of child language development. In the infancy stage (0–2 years), parental responsiveness includes recognizing and responding to babbling, which encourages the development of speech, and encouraging turn-taking in communication to help infants learn the rhythm of conversation. This responsiveness is crucial for fostering optimal language development in children.

C. Early Childhood (2–6 years)

1. Vocabulary Acquisition

The expansion of a child’s vocabulary through daily interactions is a crucial aspect of language development. Everyday conversations expose children to new words, while the role of parental modeling is also significant, as children often imitate the language patterns of their parents. These factors play a vital role in shaping the linguistic abilities of children during their early years.

2. Syntax and Grammar Development

The search results provide information on the stages of child language development, including the role of parental responsiveness in fostering optimal language development. During the early childhood stage (2–6 years), parents play a crucial role in the syntax and grammar development of their children. They help shape grammatical structures by modeling correct sentence formation and provide corrective feedback in a supportive manner, which contributes to the child’s linguistic development.

3. Parental Involvement in Literacy

During the childhood stage (2–6 years), parental involvement in literacy is crucial for fostering optimal language development. Reading aloud and storytelling expose children to narratives, which support language comprehension, while providing access to books and educational materials aids vocabulary growth. These factors play a vital role in shaping the linguistic abilities of children during their early years.

D. Middle Childhood (6–12 years)

1. Complex Language Structures

The search results provide information on the stages of child language development, including the middle childhood stage (6–12 years). During this stage, children are exposed to advanced vocabulary, which helps them learn more sophisticated language through exposure to varied content. Encouraging conversations on various topics also plays a significant role in broadening their language skills.

2. Reading and Writing Skills

The stages of child language development encompass prenatal language exposure, infancy, early childhood, middle childhood, adolescence, and various linguistic milestones. Parental involvement, such as reading to the unborn child, encouraging turn-taking in communication, and providing access to diverse reading materials, plays a crucial role in fostering optimal language development. This involvement continues across developmental stages, including supporting literacy, assisting with homework, and promoting expressive language development.

3. Communication with Teachers

The stages of child language development encompass various milestones, including prenatal language exposure, infancy, early childhood, middle childhood, and adolescence. Parental involvement is crucial across these stages, from recognizing and responding to babbling in infancy to open communication with teachers in middle childhood. This involvement includes encouraging conversations, providing access to books, and collaborating in language learning strategies.

E. Adolescence (12–18 years)

1. Abstract Language Skills

During adolescence (12–18 years), engaging in sophisticated conversations enhances abstract language skills, while encouraging critical thinking and argumentation fosters logical and persuasive expression. Parental involvement in language learning strategies, such as open communication with teachers and joint efforts to enhance language skills, continues to play a crucial role in fostering optimal language development.

2. Social and Pragmatic Language

The stages of child language development encompass various milestones, including prenatal language exposure, infancy, early childhood, middle childhood, and adolescence. Parental involvement is crucial across these stages, from recognizing and responding to babbling in infancy to open communication with teachers in middle childhood. This involvement includes encouraging conversations, providing access to books, and collaborating in language learning strategies.

3. Parental Influence on Communication Styles

The stages of child language development, from prenatal language exposure to adolescence, involve various milestones and parental roles. These include fostering early language development, supporting literacy, and encouraging advanced language skills. Parental involvement, such as recognizing and responding to babbling, open communication with teachers, and respecting individual communication styles, plays a crucial role in fostering optimal language development.

III. Phonological Development

Phonological development refers to the acquisition and refinement of speech sounds in a child’s language development journey. The process encompasses several stages and milestones, each contributing to the development of the child’s phonological system.

I. Pre-Vocal Speech Development

Pre-vocal speech development refers to the stage before a child produces recognizable speech sounds. During this stage, children develop pre-language skills, such as gestures, facial expressions, imitation, joint attention, and eye contact. The pre-linguistic stage, which is the first stage of language development, often occurs between zero and six months. During this stage, babies make cooing sounds, turn towards sounds, and give eye contact when they hear an adult speaking or making sounds. The ability to produce speech sounds starts to develop at a much younger age, and infants produce cooing sounds when they are content.

II. Reflexive Vocalization

Reflexive vocalization refers to the involuntary vocalizations produced by an infant in response to stimuli. These include non-speech sounds such as cries, grunts, sneezing, and hiccups during the first two months after birth. This stage is part of the early vocal development of infants, which progresses through various levels, including reflexive vocalizations, control of phonation, and expansion.

III. Vegetative Sounds

Vegetative sounds are sounds associated with the physiological functions of the body, such as sucking, swallowing, and breathing sounds. These sounds are part of the earliest vocalizations of infants, which include crying and reflexive sounds such as grunts, sneezing, and hiccups. The ability to produce these sounds helps infants familiarize themselves with the processes involved in later speech production. It’s important to note that the term “vegetative” is also used in medicine to describe an altered state of consciousness in which a person is not aware of and cannot meaningfully respond to their surroundings.

IV. Cooing and Laughter

Cooing and laughter are early intentional vocalizations expressing pleasure or contentment in infants. Cooing sounds are vowel-like sounds produced by infants between 6 and 8 weeks of age, while laughter is a vocalization produced in response to positive stimuli. These sounds are part of the pre-speech vocalizations of infants, which also include reflexive vocalizations such as cries, coughs, and hiccups. The ability to produce these sounds is an important milestone in the development of speech and language skills in infants.

V. Vocal Play

Vocal play is the stage during which infants, typically around 4 months of age, engage in experimentation with a variety of sounds, including rudimentary consonant-vowel syllables. This stage marks the beginning of infants’ exploration of different pitch, volume, and timbre in their vocalizations, as they start to imitate and produce a wider range of sounds.

VI. Reduplicated Babbling

Reduplicated babbling is the repetition of consonant-vowel combinations, such as “baba” and “mama,” rhythmically. This stage of vocal development typically occurs between 6 and 10 months of age and is characterized by the production of canonical syllables, which are repetitive consonant-vowel combinations. Reduplicated babbling is an important milestone in the development of speech and language skills in infants, as it marks the beginning of the production of more complex sounds and the exploration of different sound combinations.

VII. Non-Reduplicated Babbling

Non-reduplicated babbling is the stage during which infants, typically around 10 months of age, experiment with varied combinations of consonants and vowels without repetition. This stage is characterized by the production of non-canonical syllables, which are non-repetitive consonant-vowel combinations. Non-reduplicated babbling is an important milestone in the development of speech and language skills in infants, as it marks the beginning of the production of more complex sounds and the exploration of different sound combinations.

VIII. Invented Words

In the context of infant vocal development, “invented words” refer to the creation of novel words or sounds to convey meaning. An example of this is when an infant uses a unique sound to refer to a specific object or person. This process reflects the early stages of intentional communication and language development in infants. The term “invent” is typically associated with the creation of something new through the use of imagination or experimentation. In the linguistic context, the creation of new words or sounds, such as neologisms, plays a significant role in the evolution of language and communication.

IX. Natural Order of Sound Acquisition

The natural order of sound acquisition refers to the predictable sequence in which children acquire speech sounds. The full range of vowels in the native language is produced before the full range of consonants, and consonants are typically added in a specific sequence of manner, with nasals and glides being acquired before fricatives and affricates. The acquisition of speech sounds follows a developmental order, with simpler sounds like /p/ and /m/ being acquired before more complex sounds like /θ/ and /r/. It’s important to note that children vary in their development of speech and language skills, and the natural order of sound acquisition is a general guideline rather than a strict rule.

X. Articulatory Process

The natural order of sound acquisition in children follows a predictable sequence, with simpler sounds like /p/ and /m/ acquired before more complex sounds like /θ/ and /r/. This process is part of the articulatory development, which involves the coordination of speech organs to produce speech sounds. Infants also go through various stages of vocal development, including reflexive vocalization, vegetative sounds, cooing, babbling, and the creation of invented words to convey meaning. These stages mark the progression from involuntary vocalizations to intentional communication and language development.

XI. Substitution

In the context of speech and language, “substitution” refers to the replacement of one sound with another, such as saying “wabbit” for “rabbit.” This term is also used in mathematics and logic to denote the replacement of one entity with another of equal value.

Stopping: Substitution of a stop consonant for a fricative or affricate (e.g., “toop” for “soup”).

Fronting: Substitution of a front sound for a back sound (e.g., “tat” for “cat”).

Gliding: Substitution of a glide consonant for a liquid consonant (e.g., “wabbit” for “rabbit”).

Denasalization: Loss of nasal quality in nasal sounds (e.g., pronouncing “n” as “d”).

Deletion and Simplification of Syllables: Omission of sounds or syllables in words (e.g., “nana” for “banana”) and simplification of complex syllable structures (e.g., “ba” for “banana”).

XII. Assimilation

Assimilation is a phonological process in which one sound in a word becomes more like a neighboring sound, making it easier to pronounce. This process occurs when sounds in a word change to match nearby sounds, such as “red ball” becoming “reb ball”. Assimilation can be anticipatory, where a speech sound is influenced in anticipation of the sound that’s about to be spoken after it, or perseveratory, where a sound is influenced by properties persevering.

XIII. Syllable Simplification

Syllable simplification refers to the process of simplifying complex syllable structures in speech. This process involves altering the structure of a particular word, specifically the structure of the syllables that make up the word. An example of syllable simplification is saying “tat” for “cat” to simplify the consonant cluster. Syllable simplification is a common phonological process in the development of speech in children, and it is one of the five main structural simplifications or syllable structure processes.

In summary, phonological development in a child progresses through various stages, from reflexive vocalizations to the intentional use of speech sounds and the refinement of articulatory processes. The natural order of sound acquisition and the development of phonological processes contribute to a child’s ability to produce and understand speech.

IV. Vocabulary Development of Childhood

The mental lexicon of children expands as they learn and use new words. Parental input significantly influences the richness of a child’s vocabulary. Regular exposure to diverse words at home can lead to an extensive vocabulary in children. This process is essential for the development of language and literacy skills necessary for success in school. The quantity and quality of words used by adults in a child’s environment play a significant role in vocabulary development. Children learn what they hear most, and they need to learn diverse examples of words and language structures to develop their vocabulary skills. The acquisition of native language phonology begins in the womb and continues through the teenage years, with infants being able to extract meaningful distinctions in the language they are exposed to.

  1. Word Spurts: Word spurts, also known as vocabulary bursts or language explosions, refer to sudden and rapid increases in a child’s vocabulary acquisition. This phenomenon often occurs around the age of 18 to 24 months when children experience significant growth in their ability to learn and use new words. Word spurts are characterized by a noticeable increase in the number of words a child understands and produces within a relatively short period.

2.

The Mental Lexicon and Meaning: The mental lexicon refers to the internalized mental dictionary or repository of words and their meanings that individuals possess. This section explores how children organize and store words in their mental lexicon, as well as how they attribute meanings to words based on their experiences and interactions with the world. It discusses the role of semantic networks and conceptual categories in shaping children’s understanding of word meanings.

Overextension Errors: Overextension errors occur when children apply a word to a broader range of objects or contexts than is appropriate based on its conventional meaning. For example, a child might use the word “dog” to refer to all four-legged animals or even stuffed animal toys. Overextension errors are a common feature of early language development and reflect children’s attempts to make sense of the world by applying familiar words to new situations.

Underextension Errors: Underextension errors, on the other hand, occur when children restrict the use of a word to a narrower range of objects or contexts than is appropriate based on its conventional meaning. For example, a child might use the word “ball” only to refer to a specific ball they are familiar with, rather than extending it to other objects that also qualify as balls.

Underextension errors can indicate that children are still in the process of refining their understanding of word meanings and categories.

Each of these topics contributes to our understanding of how children acquire and develop their vocabulary during early childhood. By exploring the processes involved in vocabulary development, including word spurts, the mental lexicon and meaning, and common language errors such as overextension and underextension, researchers and educators can gain insights into the dynamic and complex nature of language acquisition in children.

V. Morphological Development of Childhood

Morphological development in children occurs in stages. Between 12 and 26 months, children connect subjects with verbs and actions with objects. By 31 to 34 months, they start using auxiliary verbs and conjunctions. As they progress through the school-age years, they develop metalinguistic competence, becoming more aware of the rules and features of language. This development is crucial for the acquisition of grammatical structures and the refinement of verb tense understanding, as seen in the example of a child initially saying “I runned” and later correcting it to “I ran”. The acquisition of morphemes and grammatical structures is a fundamental aspect of language development, impacting a child’s ability to express themselves effectively and understand the nuances of language. Typical language development involves unconscious use of morphology. Very young children produce overgeneralizations, such as “buyed” (instead of “bought”). The production of these errors suggests a gradual development in understanding the rules of morphology. Children gradually refine their knowledge of derivational morphology, with significant improvement typically occurring between grades 4 and 8. Vocabulary and grammar develop in synchrony during the first years of a child’s life. Vocabulary can help children to learn grammar and vice versa. It is also important that children hear rich and diverse language to develop their vocabulary skills.

  1. Overgeneralization: Overgeneralization occurs when children apply grammatical rules or morphological patterns inappropriately, resulting in errors in language production. For example, a child might use the past tense marker “-ed” with irregular verbs (e.g., “runned” instead of “ran”) or apply plural marking to irregular nouns (e.g., “foots” instead of “feet”). Overgeneralization reflects children’s attempts to apply general linguistic rules and patterns to new words or contexts, even when those rules do not accurately reflect the language’s conventions.

Natural Order of Morpheme Acquisition: The natural order of morpheme acquisition refers to the sequence in which children acquire different morphological markers and structures as they develop language proficiency. This sequence is generally consistent across languages and follows a predictable pattern. For example, English-speaking children typically acquire present progressive (-ing) and plural (-s) morphemes before past tense (-ed) and possessive (-’s) morphemes. Understanding the natural order of morpheme acquisition provides valuable insights into the stages and milestones of morphological development in children.

Derivation and Compounding: Derivation and compounding are two common processes through which new words are formed in language:

Derivation: Derivation involves adding affixes (prefixes, suffixes, infixes) to existing words to create new words with modified meanings or grammatical functions. For example, adding the prefix “un-” to the word “happy” forms the derived word “unhappy,” which means “not happy.” Derivation allows speakers to create new words by modifying or extending the meanings of existing ones.

  • Compounding: Compounding involves combining two or more existing words to create a new word with a distinct meaning. For example, combining the words “tooth” and “brush” forms the compound word “toothbrush,” which refers to a tool used for cleaning teeth. Compounding allows speakers to express complex concepts or ideas by combining simpler words into single lexical units.

Understanding derivation and compounding is essential for comprehending the morphological structure of words and analyzing how new words are formed in a language. These processes contribute to the richness and flexibility of vocabulary in linguistic communication.

By exploring morphological development, overgeneralization, the natural order of morpheme acquisition, derivation, and compounding, researchers gain insights into the intricate processes involved in language acquisition and the formation of words in human languages. These topics are fundamental to understanding the structure, development, and use of language across different contexts and populations.

VI. Syntactic Development of Childhood

During the stages of language development, children progress from the one-word stage to more complex sentence structures. The one-word stage, typically occurring between 10 and 12 months, is when children say their first words. The two-word stage, also known as telegraphic speech, begins around 1.5 to 2.5 years of age, during which children start combining single words to produce two-word utterances. For example, they may say “want juice” or “doggie bite.” The telegraphic stage occurs around the age of 2 1/2 years, during which children begin stringing more than two words together, resembling the way of writing that used to be used in telegrams. This stage marks the transition from using simple pairings of words to more complex sentence structures, as children begin to convey more nuanced ideas.

1.The One-Word Stage: The one-word stage, also known as the holophrastic stage, is an early phase of language development typically observed in infants between the ages of 10 and 18 months. During this stage, children produce single words to convey entire thoughts, ideas, or requests. These early words, known as holophrases, serve as placeholders for more complex linguistic structures and may be accompanied by gestures or intonation to convey additional meaning. While limited in form, holophrases allow infants to communicate basic needs, preferences, and observations as they begin to develop language skills.

2. The Two-Word Stage: The two-word stage marks a significant milestone in syntactic development, typically occurring around 18 to 24 months of age. During this stage, children begin to combine two words to form simple, two-word utterances known as telegraphic speech. These utterances typically follow a subject-verb or verb-object pattern and convey basic relationships or actions (e.g., “more milk,” “big dog”). While still simplified compared to adult speech, telegraphic utterances represent an important step toward more complex syntactic structures and language production.

3. Telegraphic Stage: The telegraphic stage is a phase of language development characterized by the use of short, grammatically simplified utterances that omit non-essential elements such as articles, prepositions, and auxiliary verbs. This stage typically occurs during the early stages of syntactic development, around the ages of 24 to 30 months. Telegraphic speech reflects children’s growing understanding of grammatical rules and syntactic structures, allowing them to express more complex ideas and relationships through increasingly sophisticated language forms.

Understanding syntactic development, including the one-word stage, the two-word stage, and the telegraphic stage, provides valuable insights into the progression of language acquisition and the emergence of grammatical competence in early childhood. These stages represent important milestones in the development of syntactic skills, laying the foundation for more advanced language abilities and communicative competence as children continue to grow and mature linguistically.

VII. Advanced Development of Childhood

During adolescence, advanced language skills, critical thinking, and persuasive expression develop. Students with typically developing advanced language skills are able to use complex sentences in both their oral and written language. They can also produce written stories that follow story grammar rules and make correct inferences from written text. Additionally, adolescents develop the ability to understand and use figurative language, such as metaphors, similes, and idioms. They also gain the capacity to comprehend and use expository text, which requires good metalinguistic awareness skills. This progression from simple to complex language structures is a fundamental aspect of language development during adolescence, enabling individuals to discuss complex topics and express nuanced ideas.

Advanced language development in childhood refers to the acquisition and refinement of complex linguistic skills beyond the basic level. It encompasses the ability to comprehend and produce increasingly sophisticated language structures, vocabulary, and discourse in various contexts. Here are some key aspects of advanced language development in childhood:

1. Morphological Complexity: Children demonstrate proficiency in using a wide range of morphological markers and rules to convey precise meanings. They acquire more complex morphological structures, such as irregular past tense forms, comparative and superlative adjectives, and derivational suffixes.

2. Syntactic Sophistication: Advanced language learners demonstrate mastery of complex syntactic structures, including embedded clauses, passive constructions, relative clauses, and coordination of multiple clauses within a sentence. They also exhibit syntactic flexibility and creativity in constructing sentences of varying lengths and complexities.

3. Lexical Diversity and Precision: Children expand their vocabulary repertoire to include a broad range of words from different semantic categories, including nouns, verbs, adjectives, adverbs, and prepositions. They acquire specialized vocabulary related to specific topics of interest or academic domains and demonstrate precision in word choice to convey nuanced meanings.

4. Pragmatic Competence: Advanced language learners develop pragmatic skills to effectively use language in social interactions and communication. They demonstrate awareness of conversational norms, turn-taking strategies, politeness conventions, and the ability to adapt their language use to different contexts, audiences, and purposes.

5. Discourse Coherence and Cohesion: Children become proficient in organizing and structuring their language to create coherent and cohesive discourse. They use cohesive devices, such as pronouns, conjunctions, lexical repetition, and discourse markers, to establish logical relationships between ideas and maintain coherence across sentences and paragraphs.

6. Metalinguistic Awareness: Advanced language learners develop metalinguistic awareness, which involves the ability to reflect on and manipulate language as an object of thought. They demonstrate understanding of language rules, conventions, and structures, as well as the ability to analyze and evaluate language use in context.

7. Narrative and Expository Skills: Children acquire the ability to produce narrative and expository texts that adhere to conventional organizational patterns and rhetorical structures. They demonstrate proficiency in storytelling, recounting personal experiences, summarizing information, and presenting arguments or explanations in written and oral forms.

8. Literacy Development: Advanced language development is closely intertwined with literacy development, as children continue to refine their reading and writing skills. They demonstrate fluency in reading comprehension, decoding, and word recognition, as well as proficiency in writing for different purposes and audiences.

VIII. Communicative Development of Childhood

Children’s communicative development involves not only expressing themselves but also understanding and responding appropriately to others. As they engage in social interactions, they learn to adapt their communication style to suit different audiences or contexts, demonstrating growing communicative abilities. This development aligns with the pragmatic component of language, emphasizing the intention, purpose, or function of their communicative efforts within social contexts. Additionally, parents and caregivers play a significant role in encouraging communication and language development in children, as they provide the necessary environment and support for the growth of language and communicative competencies.

IX. Factors Affecting First Language Acquisition of Childhood

Various factors can impact language development in children, including internal and external factors, child-directed speech, feedback, and recasts. Additionally, exposure, socioeconomic status, and parental education can also play a significant role in language acquisition. Children in language-rich environments with educated parents may have advantages in language acquisition. Conversational skill development is also an important aspect of language development, including childhood egocentrism, collective monologues, and private speech. Other factors that influence language development include motivation, support at home, prior linguistic knowledge, learning environment, teaching strategies, comprehensible input, and student interest.

X. Atypical Language Development of Childhood

Various factors can impact language development in children, including deafness, blindness, intellectual disabilities, Down syndrome, autism spectrum disorders, Specific Language Impairment (SLI), and language processing disorders. Children with SLI may struggle to form grammatically correct sentences compared to their peers. Other signs of atypical language development include a small vocabulary for age, trouble understanding simple language, and difficulty recalling sounds in new words. Early identification of atypical language development is crucial for effective intervention and support. Children with atypical language development may benefit from speech therapy, language intervention, and other forms of support to help them develop their language and communication skills.

1. Deafness: Deafness refers to partial or complete hearing loss, which can significantly impact a child’s ability to acquire spoken language. Children who are deaf may benefit from early intervention with sign language, hearing aids, cochlear implants, or other assistive devices to support their communication and language development.

2. Blindness: Blindness involves the loss of vision, which can affect a child’s ability to perceive and interact with the world. Children who are blind may rely on alternative methods of communication, such as braille, auditory cues, tactile stimulation, and assistive technologies, to support their language and cognitive development.

3. Intellectual Disabilities: Intellectual disabilities are characterized by limitations in intellectual functioning and adaptive behavior. Children with intellectual disabilities may have difficulties with language acquisition, problem-solving, social skills, and independent living skills. Early intervention and individualized support services can help optimize their development and learning outcomes.

4. Down Syndrome: Down syndrome is a genetic disorder caused by the presence of an extra copy of chromosome 21. Children with Down syndrome typically have intellectual disabilities, distinctive physical features, and may experience delays in language development. Early intervention programs that target speech and language skills, along with educational and behavioral interventions, can support their overall development.

5. Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD): ASD is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by challenges with social communication and interaction, restricted interests, and repetitive behaviors. Children with ASD may have difficulties with language development, including delays in speech and language acquisition, echolalia (repetitive speech), and pragmatic language deficits. Early intervention, behavioral therapies, and speech-language therapy are key components of comprehensive support for children with ASD.

6. Developmental Language Disorders: Developmental language disorders refer to impairments in the acquisition and use of language that are not attributable to neurological or sensory deficits. These disorders may manifest as difficulties with expressive or receptive language skills, phonological processing, grammar, vocabulary, and pragmatic language abilities. Speech-language therapy and educational interventions tailored to the child’s specific needs are essential for supporting language development in children with developmental language disorders.

In addressing the atypical development of childhood, it’s crucial to adopt a holistic and individualized approach that considers the unique strengths, challenges, and needs of each child. Early identification, comprehensive assessment, family-centered interventions, and interdisciplinary collaboration among healthcare professionals, educators, and caregivers are essential for promoting positive outcomes and enhancing the quality of life for children with atypical developmental conditions.

XI. Bilingual First Language Acquisition

Bilingual first language acquisition (BFLA) occurs when a child is exposed to and learns two languages from birth. This process can take the form of simultaneous bilingualism, where the child acquires two languages at the same time from birth, or sequential bilingualism, where the child learns a second language after already having a solid foundation in the first. Research has shown that the one-person-one-language approach can lead to successful acquisition of two languages. However, the science of bilingualism is a young field, and definitive answers to many questions are not yet available. Factors influencing bilingual first language acquisition include parental input, language dominance, and the child’s learning environment. Additionally, the child’s ability to effortlessly switch between languages depending on the context is a common example of bilingual language use in a household. The development of language skills in a bilingual or multilingual context is an area of growing interest and research, with a focus on understanding the factors that contribute to successful bilingual language acquisition in children.

How FLA differs from SLA (Second Language acquisition)?

First Language Acquisition (FLA) and Second Language Acquisition (SLA) differ in various ways, including the context, timing, processes, and outcomes of language learning. Here are some key differences between FLA and SLA:

1. Timing:— FLA: Begins at birth and continues throughout early childhood.— SLA: Typically begins after the critical period for language acquisition, which extends from infancy to puberty. SLA can occur at any age, from childhood to adulthood.

2. Context:— FLA: Occurs naturally within the home environment, where children are exposed to language through interactions with caregivers, family members, and peers.— SLA: Often takes place in formal educational settings, language classes, immersion programs, or naturalistic environments where the target language is spoken.

3. Input:— FLA: Children are exposed to rich and consistent language input from native speakers in their immediate environment.— SLA: Learners may have limited or variable exposure to the target language, and the quality and quantity of input may vary depending on the learning context and opportunities for language practice.

4. Cognitive Factors:— FLA: Language acquisition occurs in conjunction with cognitive and motor development, guided by innate language learning mechanisms.— SLA: Learners bring pre-existing cognitive abilities, metalinguistic awareness, and learning strategies to the language learning process. SLA may involve conscious learning strategies and explicit instruction.

5. Grammatical Development:— FLA: Children acquire grammatical structures and rules of their first language in a systematic and predictable sequence, following developmental milestones.— SLA: Learners may experience variability in the acquisition of grammatical structures, influenced by factors such as age, proficiency level, first language background, and learning context.

6. Pragmatic Knowledge:— FLA: Children develop pragmatic competence through exposure to social interactions and communicative exchanges in their first language community.— SLA: Learners may encounter challenges in acquiring pragmatic norms and sociocultural conventions of the target language, particularly in understanding idiomatic expressions, humor, and nonverbal cues.

7. Psychological Factors:— FLA: Language acquisition is integral to identity formation, socialization, and emotional development in childhood.— SLA: Learners may experience affective factors such as anxiety, motivation, and self-confidence, which can influence their language learning experiences and outcomes.

8. Ultimate Attainment:— FLA: Typically results in native-like proficiency and automaticity in the first language, with mastery of linguistic and communicative competence.— SLA: Maybe result in varying degrees of proficiency, ranging from basic conversational skills to near-native or native-like proficiency, depending on individual factors, age of acquisition, exposure, and learning context.

In summary, FLA and SLA differ in terms of onset, context, input, cognitive processes, grammatical development, pragmatic knowledge, psychological factors, and ultimate attainment. While FLA is characterized by naturalistic language learning within the home environment from birth, SLA involves conscious language learning efforts in diverse educational and social contexts, typically beginning after the critical period for language acquisition.

Conclusion

The process of first language acquisition in children is a remarkable and complex journey that unfolds in several key stages. From the one-word (holophrastic) stage to the multi-word stage, children gradually acquire and produce real words of their native languages. This cognitive development, although not taught, is heavily dependent on the language-rich environment provided by parents and caregivers. The stages of language acquisition occur in a universal order for all children, reflecting the innate capacity for language learning. Parental involvement is crucial, as children learn first words and more from their speech-rich environment. The verbal environment significantly influences language learning, and children benefit from exposure to diverse language inputs. Additionally, the importance of continued parental involvement across developmental stages cannot be overstated. Parents play a vital role in fostering optimal language development by engaging in frequent, meaningful conversations with their children and providing a supportive language-rich environment. This involvement is particularly important in bilingual or multilingual households, where children may effortlessly switch between languages depending on the context. Overall, the factors that influence language development, such as exposure, socioeconomic status, and parental education, underscore the significance of parental input in shaping children’s language skills and abilities. Therefore, understanding the key stages of language acquisition and the practical implications for parents in supporting language development is essential for promoting effective communication and literacy skills in children.



3 views0 comments

Recent Posts

See All

Comments


bottom of page